New
Sep 16, 2012 9:13 PM
#1
"The time had come for Trapp to address the men and inform them of the assignment the battalion had received... Pale and nervous, with choking voice and tears in his eyes, Trapp visibly fought to control himself as he spoke. The battalion, he said plaintively, had to perform a frightfully unpleasant task. This assignment was not to his liking, indeed it was highly regrettable, but the orders came from the highest authorities...The Jews had instigated the American boycott that had damaged Germany...There were Jews in the village of Jozefow who were involved with the partisans, he explained...The battalion had now been order to round up these Jews. The male Jews of working age were to be separated and taken to a work camp. The remaining Jews---The women, children, and elderly---were to be shot on the spot by the battalion... if any of the older men (keep in mind everyone here is past military age, this is a killing police hes talking to-they're like ~40-60 most likely)-among them did not feel up to the task that lay before him, he could step out..." -Ordinary Men Out of the 500 men that were assigned to slaughter the village of ~1800, only about twelve decided against it. One was left behind in charge of guarding the barracks. What makes retired men-just like you and me-most with no affiliation to the nazi army slaughter the 1.5k men women and children in such a horrific way-and what is even more interesting-that the commanding officer gives the men the opportunity to drop the assignment-Trapp is basically the average German-it is safe to assume that almost all of the men would have faced similar inner turmoil while making a decision-and yet, why is it that only 12 men drop the assignment? Trapp gave explicit directions to the men what they would do (instantly kill women, children and the sick/elderly, bring the men to concentration camps) and a small percentage gave up the assignment What is the ordinary man? The man who would take the opportunity to be reassigned or the one who would take part in killing over a thousand people? The Nazi Party may have had plurality-but that is far from having a majority- most people at the time were anti-nazi party or ambivalent-What makes men like those kill over 15 million people in such a short time span? If you take the statement "Germany was pregnant with murder" and dissect it, can you get your answer? Germany had a crumbling economy with hyper-inflation-a broken government-anti-antisemitism- anti-European sentiment-nationalism to almost levels of fascism-we can rephrase this-Was Germany so fucked during WW2 that it had no choice but to kill? This raises up a few more questions-What is the whole? What is society vs. Individualism? Can the common mindset infiltrate the mind and break down individualism? A lot of people said that Hitler was the leader who instigated the mass murders-I would say no-the hate that the German people had would raise into power another man another party to the job- Are they ordinary men? What is it to be an ordinary man? |
~"The place to improve the world is first in one's own heart and head and hands." (Pirsig) |
Sep 16, 2012 9:34 PM
#2
"If Hitler Asked You to Electrocute a Stranger, Would You? Probably" by Philip Meyer, Esquire (1970) In the beginning, Stanley Milgram was worried about the Nazi problem. He doesn't worry much about the Nazis anymore. He worries about you and me, and, perhaps, himself a little bit too. Stanley Milgram is a social psychologist, and when he began his career at Yale University in 1960 he had a plan to prove, scientifically, that Germans are different. The Germans-are-different hypothesis has been used by historians, such as William L. Shirer, to explain the systematic destruction of the Jews by the Third Reich. One madman could decide to destroy the Jews and even create a master plan for getting it done. But to implement it on the scale that Hitler did meant that thousands of other people had to go along with the scheme and help to do the work. The Shirer thesis, which Milgram set out to test, is that Germans have a basic character flaw which explains the whole thing, and this flaw is a readiness to obey authority without question, no matter what outrageous acts the authority commands. The appealing thing about this theory is that it makes those of us who are not Germans feel better about the whole business. Obviously, you and I are not Hitler, and it seems equally obvious that we would never do Hitler's dirty work for him. But now, because of Stanley Milgram, we are compelled to wonder. Milgram developed a laboratory experiment which provided a systematic way to measure obedience. His plan was to try it out in New Haven on Americans and then go to Germany and try it out on Germans. He was strongly motivated by scientific curiosity, but there was also some moral content in his decision to pursue this line of research, which was in turn colored by his own Jewish background. If he could show that Germans are more obedient than Americans, he could then vary the conditions of the experiment and try to find out just what it is that makes some people more obedient than others. With this understanding, the world might, conceivably, be just a little bit better. But he never took his experiment to Germany. He never took it any farther than Bridgeport. The first finding, also the most unexpected and disturbing finding, was that we Americans are an obedient people: not blindly obedient, and not blissfully obedient, just obedient. "I found so much obedience," says Milgram softly, a little sadly, "I hardly saw the need for taking the experiment to Germany." There is something of the theater director in Milgram, and his technique, which he learned from one of the old masters in experimental psychology, Solomon Asch, is to stage a play with every line rehearsed, every prop carefully selected, and everybody an actor except one person. That one person is the subject of the experiment. The subject, of course, does not know he is in a play. He thinks he is in real life. The value of this technique is that the experimenter, as though he were God, can change a prop here, vary a line there, and see how the subject responds. Milgram eventually had to change a lot of the script just to get people to stop obeying. They were obeying so much, the experiment wasn't working--it was like trying to measure oven temperature with a freezer thermometer. The experiment worked like this: If you were an innocent subject in Milgram's melodrama, you read an ad in the newspaper or received one in the mail asking for volunteers for an educational experiment. The job would take about an hour and pay $4.50. So you make an appointment and go to an old Romanesque stone structure on High Street with the imposing name of the Yale Interaction Laboratory. It looks something like a broadcasting studio. Inside, you meet a young, crew-cut man in a laboratory coat who says he is Jack Williams, the experimenter. There is another citizen, fiftyish, Irish face, an accountant, a little overweight, and very mild and harmless looking. This other citizen seems nervous and plays with his hat while the two of you sit in chairs side by side and are told that the $4.50 checks are yours no matter what happens. Then you listen to Jack Williams explain the experiment. It is about learning, says Jack Williams in a quiet, knowledgeable way. Science does not know much about the conditions under which people learn and this experiment is to find out about negative reinforcement. Negative reinforcement is getting punished when you do something wrong, as opposed to positive reinforcement which is getting rewarded when you do something right. The negative reinforcement in this case is electric shock. You notice a book on the table, titled, The Teaching-Learning Process, and you assume that this has something to do with the experiment. Then Jack Williams takes two pieces of paper, puts them in a hat, and shakes them up. One piece of paper is supposed to say, "Teacher" and the other, "Learner." Draw one and you will see which you will be. The milder looking accountant draws one, holds it close to his vest like a poker player, looks at it, and says, "Learner." You look at yours. It says, "Teacher." You do not know that the drawing is rigged, and both slips say "Teacher." The experimenter beckons to the mild-mannered "learner." "Want to step right in here and have a seat, please?" he says. "You can leave your coat on the back of that chair...roll up your right sleeve, please. Now what I want to do is strap down your arms to avoid excessive movement on your part during the experiment. This electrode is connected to the shock generator in the next room. And this electrode paste," he says, squeezing some stuff out of a plastic bottle and putting it on the man's arm, "is to provide a good contact and to avoid a blister or burn. Are there any questions now before we go into the next room?" You don't have any, but the strapped-in "learner" does. "I do think I should say this," says the learner. "About two years ago, I was in the veterans' hospital...they detected a heart condition. Nothing serious, but as long as I'm having these shocks, how strong are they--how dangerous are they?" Williams, the experimenter, shakes his head casually. "Oh, no," he says. "Although they may be painful, they're not dangerous. Anything else?" Nothing else. And so you play the game. The game is for you to read a series of word pairs: for example, blue-girl, nice-day, fat-neck. When you finish the list, you read just the first word in each pair and then a multiple-choice list of four other words, including the second word of the pair. The learner, from his remote, strapped-in position, pushes one of four switches to indicate which of the four answers he thinks is the right one. If he gets it right, nothing happens and you go on to the next one. If he gets it wrong, you push a switch that buzzes and gives him an electric shock. And then you go on to the next word. You start with 15 volts and increase the number of volts by 15 for each wrong answer. The control board goes from 15 volts on one end to 450 volts on the other. So that you know what you are doing, you get a test shock yourself, at 45 volts. It hurts. To further keep you aware of what you are doing to that man in there, the board has verbal descriptions of the shock levels, ranging from "Slight Shock� at the left-hand side, through "Intense Shock" in the middle to "Danger: Severe Shock� toward the far right. Finally, at the very end, under 435- and 450-volt switches, there are three ambiguous X's. If, at any point, you hesitate, Mr. Williams calmly tells you to go on. If you still hesitate, he tells you again. Except for some terrifying details, which will be explained in a moment, this is the experiment. The object is to find the shock level at which you disobey the experimenter and refuse to pull the switch. When Stanley Milgram first wrote this script, he took it to 14 Yale psychology majors and asked them what they thought would happen. He put it this way: Out of one hundred persons in the teacher�s predicament, how would their break-off points be distributed along the 15- to 450-volt scale? They thought a few would break off very early, most would quit someplace in the middle, and a few would go all the way to the end. The highest estimate of the number out of 100 who would go all the way to the end was three. Milgram then informally polled some of his fellow scholars in the psychology department. They agreed that very few would go to the end. Milgram thought so too. "I'll tell you quite frankly," he says, "before I began this experiment, before any shock generator was built, I thought that most people would break off at 'Strong Shock' or �Very Strong Shock.' You would get only a very, very small proportion of people going out to the end of the shock generator, and they would constitute a pathological fringe." In his pilot experiments, Milgram used Yale students as subjects. Each of them pushed the shock switches, one by one, all the way to the end of the board. So he rewrote the script to include some protests from the learner. At first, they were mild, gentlemanly, Yalie protests, but "it didn't seem to have as much effect as I thought it would or should,� Milgram recalls. "So we had more violent protestation on the part of the person getting the shock. All of the time, of course, what we were trying to do was not to create a macabre situation, but simply to generate disobedience. And that was one of the first findings. This was not only a technical deficiency of the experiment, that we didn�t get disobedience. It really was the first finding: that obedience would be much greater than we had assumed it would be and disobedience would be uch more difficult than we had assumed." As it turned out, the situation did become rather macabre. The only meaningful way to generate disobedience was to have the victim protest with great anguish, noise, and vehemence. The protests were tape-recorded so that all the teachers ordinarily would hear the same sounds and nuances, and they started with a grunt at 75 volts, proceeded through a "Hey, that really hurts,� at 125 volts, got desperate with, "I can't stand the pain, don�t do that," at 180 volts, reached complaints of heart trouble at 195, an agonized scream at 285, a refusal to answer at 315, and only heart-rending, ominous silence after that. Still, 65 percent of the subjects, 20- to 50-year-old American males, everyday, ordinary people, like you and me, obediently kept pushing those levers in the belief that they were shocking the mild-mannered learner, whose name was Mr. Wallace, and who was chosen for the role because of his innocent appearance, all the way up to 450 volts. Milgram was now getting enough disobedience so that he had something he could measure. The next step was to vary the circumstances to see what would encourage or discourage obedience. There seemed very little left in the way of discouragement. The victim was already screaming at the top of his lungs and feigning a heart attack. So whatever new impediment to obedience reached the brain of the subject had to travel by some route other than the ear. Milgram thought of one. He put the learner in the same room with the teacher. He stopped strapping the learner's hand down. He rewrote the script so that at 150 volts the learner took his hand off the shock plate and declared that he wanted out of the experiment. He rewrote the script some more so that the experimenter then told the teacher to grasp the learner's hand and physically force it down on the plate to give Mr. Wallace his unwanted electric shock. "I had the feeling that very few people would go on at that point, if any," Milgram says. "I thought that would be the limit of obedience that you would find in the laboratory." It wasn't. Although seven years have now gone by, Milgram still remembers the first person to walk into the laboratory in the newly rewritten script. He was a construction worker, a very short man. "He was so small," says Milgram, "that when he sat on the chair in front of the shock generator, his feet didn�t reach the floor. When the experimenter told him to push the victim�s hand down and give the shock, he turned to the experimenter, and he turned to the victim, his elbow went up, he fell down on the hand of the victim, his feet kind of tugged to one side, and he said, 'Like this, boss?' Zzumph!" The experiment was played out to its bitter end. Milgram tried it with 40 different subjects. And 30 percent of them obeyed the experimenter and kept on obeying. "The protests of the victim were strong and vehement, he was screaming his guts out, he refused to participate, and you had to physically struggle with him in order to get his hand down on the shock generator," Milgram remembers. But 12 our of 40 did it. Milgram took his experiment out of New Haven. Not to Germany, just 20 miles down the road to Bridgeport. Maybe, he reasoned, the people obeyed because of the prestigious setting of Yale University. If they couldn't trust a learning center that had been there for two centuries, whom could they trust? So he moved the experiment to an untrustworthy setting. The new setting was a suite of three rooms in a run-down office building in Bridgeport. The only identification was a sign with a fictitious name: "Research Associates of Bridgeport." Questions about professional connections got only vague answers about "research for industry." Obedience was less in Bridgeport. Forty-eight percent of the subjects stayed for the maximum shock, compared to 65 percent at Yale. But this was enough to prove that far more than Yale�s prestige was behind the obedient behavior. For more than seven years now, Stanley Milgram has been trying to figure out what makes ordinary American citizens so obedient. The most obvious answer--that people are mean, nasty, brutish, and sadistic--won�t do. The subjects who gave the shocks to Mr. Wallace to the end of the board did not enjoy it. They groaned, protested, fidgeted, argued, and in some cases, were seized by fits of nervous, agitated giggling. "They even try to get out of it," says Milgram, "but they are somehow engaged in something from which they cannot liberate themselves. They are locked into a structure, and they do not have the skills or inner resources to disengage themselves." Milgram, because he mistakenly had assumed that he would have trouble getting people to obey the orders to shock Mr. Wallace, went to a lot of trouble to create a realistic situation. There was crew-cut Jack Williams and his gray laboratory coat. Not white, which might denote a medical technician, but ambiguously authoritative gray. Then there was the book on the table, and the other appurtenances of the laboratory which emitted the silent message that things were being performed here in the name of science, and were therefore great and good. But the nicest touch of all was the shock generator. When Milgram started out, he had only a $300 grant from the Higging Fund of Yale University. Later he got more ample support from the National Science Foundation, but in the beginning he had to create this authentic-looking machine with very scarce resources except for his own imagination. So he went to New York and roamed around the electronic shops until he found some little black switches at Lafayette Radio for a dollar apiece. He bought 30 of them. The generator was a metal box, about the size of a small footlocker, and he drilled the 30 holes for the 30 switches himself in a Yale machine shop. But the fine detail was left to professional industrial engravers. So he ended up with a splendid-looking control panel dominated by the row of switches, each labeled with its voltage, and each having its own red light that flashed on when the switch was pulled. Other things happened when a switch was pulled. Besides the zzumphing noise, a blue light labeled "voltage energizer" went on, and a needle on a dial labeled "voltage" flicked from left to right. Relays inside the box clicked. Finally, in the upper left-hand corner of the control panel was this inscription, engraved in precise block letters: SHOCK GENERATOR TYPE ZLB DYSON INSTRUMENT COMPANY WALTHAM, MASS. OUTPUT: 15 VOLTS - 450 VOLTS One day a man from the Lehigh Valley Electronics Company of Pennsylvania was passing through the laboratory, and he stopped to admire the shock generator. "This is a very fine shock generator," he said. "But who is this Dyson Instrument Company?" Milgram felt proud at that, since Dyson Instrument Company existed only in the recesses of his imagination. When you consider the seeming authenticity of the situation, you can appreciate the agony some of the subjects went through. It was pure conflict. As Milgram, explains to his students, "When a parent says, 'Don't strike old ladies,' you are learning two things: the content and also to obey authority. This experiment creates conflict between the two elements. Here is a partial transcription from one of the experiments. The naive �teacher� is a heavyset, worried-looking man with prematurely gray hair. He is wearing a polo shirt. He is very polite. He has just pushed the 180-volt lever. A hidden camera is recording the performance. Learner (who, from the teacher's point of view is heard but not seen, an offstage voice): Ow, I can't stand the pain. Don't do that.... Teacher (pivoting around in his chair and shaking his head): I can't stand it. I'm not going to kill that man in there. You hear him hollering? Experimenter: As I told you before, the shocks may be painful, but� Teacher: But he�s hollering. He can't stand it. What's going to happen to him? Experimenter (his voice is patient, matter-of-fact): The experiment requires that you continue, Teacher. Teacher: Aaah, but, uh, I'm not going to get that man sick in there...know what I mean? Experimenter: Whether the learner likes it or not, we must go on, through all the word pairs. Teacher: I refuse to take the responsibility. He's in there hollering! Experimenter: It's absolutely essential that you continue, Teacher. Teacher (indicating the unused question): There's too many left here, I mean, Geez, if he gets them wrong, there's too many of them left. I mean who's going to take the responsibility if anything happens to that gentleman? Experimenter: I'm responsible for anything that happens to him. Continue please. Teacher: All right. (Consults list of words.) The next one's "Slow--walk, truck, dance, music." Answer, please. (A buzzing sound indicates the learner has signaled his answer.) Wrong. A hundred and ninety-five volts. "Dance." (Zzumph!) Learner: Let me out of here. My heart�s bothering me! (Teacher looks at experimenter.) Experimenter: Continue, please. Learner (screaming): Let me out of here, you have no right to keep me here. Let me out of here, let me out, my heart�s bothering me, let me out! (Teacher shakes head, pats the table nervously.) Teacher: You see, he's hollering. Hear that? Gee, I don�t know. Experimenter: The experiment requires... Teacher (interrupting): I know it does, sir, but I mean--huuh! He don't know what he's getting in for. He's up to 195 volts! (Experiment continues through 210 volts, 225 volts, 240 volts, 255 volts, 270 volts, delivered to the man in the electric chair, at which point the teacher, with evident relief, runs out of the word-pair questions.) Experimenter: You�ll have to go back to the beginning of that page and go through them again until he's learned them all correctly. Teacher: Aw, no. I'm not going to kill that man. You mean I�ve got to keep going up with the scale. No, sir. He's hollering in there. I'm not going to give him 450 volts. Experimenter: The experiment requires that you go on. Teacher: I know it does, but that man is hollering in there, sir. Experimenter (same matter-of-fact tone): As I said before, although the shocks may be painful... Teacher (interrupting): Awwww. He--he--he�s yelling in there. Experimenter: Start with "Blue," please, at the top of the page. Continue, please, Teacher. Just go ahead. Teacher (concentrating intently on list of words in front of him, as if to shut everything else out): "Blue--boy, girl, grass, hat." (Buzz indicates answer.) Correct. "Night--day, sky, job, chair." (Buzz) Wrong. The answer is "day." Two hundred and eighty-five volts. (Zzumph!) Learner: Ohhhuhhoohhh! Experimenter: Continue, please. Teacher: "Fat--man, lady, tub, neck." Answer, please. (Buzz) Wrong. The answer is "neck." Three hundred volts. (Zzumph!) Learner: Ohhh. I absolutely refuse to answer any more. (Shouting urgently, now) Let me out of here. You can't hold me here. Get me out. Get--me--out--of--here. Experimenter: Continue. The next word is "green," please. Teacher: "Green--grass, hat, ink, apple." (Nothing happens. No answering buzz. Just gloomy silence.) Teacher: I don't think he is going to answer. Experimenter: If the learner doesn't answer in a reasonable time, about four or five seconds, consider the answer wrong. And follow the same procedures you have been doing for wrong answers. Say "Wrong,� tell him the number of volts, give him the punishment, read him the correct answer. Continue, please. Teacher: "Green--grass, hat, ink, apple." Answer, please. (More silence. Teacher just sits there.) Experimenter: Go on with the procedure, please, Teacher. Teacher: Three hundred and fifteen volts. The answer is "ink." (Zzumph!) Learner: Ohh. Ooohh. Teacher (relieved to hear response): Next one.... Learner: I absolutely refuse to answer. Teacher: You got to. You get a shock if you don�t answer. (Experiment continues with learner refusing to answer, and finally, failing even to grunt or groan. At 375 volts, the teacher stands up. Has he had enough? Will he break off the experiment? Will he disobey?) Teacher: I think something's happened to that fellow in there. I don't get no answer. He was hollering at less voltage. Can't you check in and see if he's all right, please? Experimenter (same detached calm): Not once we�ve started. Please continue, Teacher. Teacher (sits down, sighs deeply): "Cool--day, shade, water, paint." Answer, please. Are you all right in there? Are you all right? Experimenter: Please continue, Teacher. Continue, please. (Teacher pushes lever. Zzumph!) Teacher (swiveling around in his chair): Something�s happened to that man in there. (Swiveling back) Next one. "Low--dollar, necklace, moon, paint." (Turning around again) Something�s happened to that man in there. You better check in on him, sir. He won't answer or nothing. Experimenter: Continue. Go on, please. Teacher: You accept all responsibility. Experimenter: The responsibility is mine. Correct. Please go on. (Teacher returns to his list, starts running through words as rapidly as he can read them, works through to 450 volts.) Teacher: That's that. Experimenter: Continue using the last switch on the board, please. The 450 switch for each wrong answer. Continue please. Teacher: But I don't get no anything! Experimenter: Please continue. The next word is "white." Teacher: Don't you think you should look in on him, please. Experimenter: Not once we've started the experiment. Teacher: But what if something has happened to the man? Experimenter: The experiment requires that you continue. Go on, please. Teacher: Don't the man's health mean anything? Experimenter: Whether the learner likes it or not... Teacher: What if he's dead in there? (Gestures toward the room with the electric chair.) I mean, he told me he can't stand the shock, sir. I don't mean to be rude, but I think you should look in on him. All you have to do is look in the door. I don't get no answer, no noise. Something might have happened to the gentleman in there, sir. Experimenter: We must continue. Go on, please. Teacher: You mean keep giving him what? Four hundred fifty volts, what he's got now? Experimenter: That's correct. Continue. The next word is "white." Teacher (now at a furious pace): "White--cloud, horse, rock, house." Answer please. The answer is "horse." Four hundred and fifty volts. (Zzumph!) Next word, "Bag--paint, music, clown, girl." The answer is "paint." Four hundred and fifty volts. (Zzumph!) Next work is "Short--sentence, movie..." Experimenter: Excuse me, Teacher. We'll have to discontinue the experiment. (Enter Milgram from camera's left. He has been watching from behind on-way glass.) Milgram: I�d like to ask you a few questions. (Slowly, patiently, he dehoaxes the teacher, telling him that the shocks and screams were not real.) Teacher: You mean he wasn't getting nothing? Well, I'm glad to hear that. I was getting upset there. I was getting ready to walk out. (Finally, to make sure there are no hard feelings, friendly, harmless Mr. Wallace comes out in coat and tie. Gives jovial greeting. Friendly reconciliation takes place. Experiment ends.) Subjects in the experiment were not asked to give the 450- volt shock more than three times. By that time, it seemed evident that they would go on indefinitely. "No one,� says Milgram, "who got within five shocks of the end ever broke off. By that point, he has resolved the conflict." Why do so many people resolve the conflict in favor of obedience? Milgram's theory assumes that people behave in two different operating modes as different as ice and water. He does not rely on Freud or sex or toilet-training hang-ups for this theory. All he says is that ordinarily we operate in a state of autonomy, which means we pretty much have and assert control over what we do. But in certain circumstances, we operate under what Milgram calls a state of agency (agent-- any one who acts for or in the place of another by authority from him; a substitute; a deputy--Webster�s Collegiate Dictionary). A state of agency, to Milgram, is nothing more than a frame of mind. "There�s nothing bad about it, there's nothing good about it," he says. "It's a natural circumstance of living with other people. I think of a state of agency as a real transformation of a person; if a person has different properties when he's in that state, just as water can turn to ice under certain conditions of temperature, a person can move to the state of mind that I call agency--the critical thing is that you see yourself as the instrument of the execution of another person's wishes. You do not see yourself as acting on your own. And there�s a real transformation, a real change of properties of the person." To achieve this change you have to be in a situation where there seems to be a ruling authority whose commands are relevant to some legitimate purpose; the authority�s power is not unlimited. But situations can be and have been structured to make people do unusual things, and not just in Milgram�s laboratory. The reason, says Milgram, is that no action, in and of itself, contains meaning. "The meaning always depends on your definition of the situation. Take an action like killing another person. It sounds bad.� "But then we say the other person was about to destroy a hundred children, and the only way to stop him was to kill him. Well, that sounds good. "Or, you take destroying your own life. It sounds very bad. Yet, in the Second World War, thousands of persons thought it was a good thing to destroy your own life. It was set in the proper context. You sipped some saki from a whistling cup, recited a few haiku. You said, 'May my death be as clean and as quick as the shattering of crystal.' And it almost seemed like a good, noble thing to do, to crash your kamikaze plane into an aircraft carrier. But the main thing was, the definition of what a kamikaze pilot was doing had been determined by the relevant authority. Now once you are in a state of agency, you allow the authority to determine, to define what the situation is. The meaning of your action is altered." So for most subjects in Milgrim�s laboratory experiments, the act of giving Mr. Wallace his painful shock was necessary, even though unpleasant, and besides they were doing it on behalf of somebody else and it was for science. There was still strain and conflict, of course. Most people resolved it by grimly sticking to their task and obeying. But some broke out. Milgram tried varying the conditions of the experiment to see what would help break people out of their state of agency. "The results, as seen and felt in the laboratory,� he has written, "are disturbing. They raise the possibility that human nature, or more specifically the kind of character produced in American democratic society, cannot be counted on to insulate its citizens from brutality and inhumane treatment at the direction of malevolent authority. A substantial proportion of people do what they are told to do, irrespective of the content of the act and without limitation of conscience, so long as they perceive that the command comes from a legitimate authority. If, in this study, an anonymous experimenter can successfully command adults to subdue a 50-year-old man and force on him painful electric shocks against his protest, one can only wonder what government, with its vastly greater authority and prestige, can command of its subjects." This is a nice statement, but it falls short of summing up the full meaning of Milgram's work. It leaves some questions still unanswered. The first question is this: Should we really be surprised and alarmed that people obey? Wouldn't it be even more alarming if they all refused to obey? Without obedience to a relevant ruling authority there could not be a civil society. An without a civil society, as Thomas Hobbes pointed out in the seventeenth century, we would live in a condition of war, "of every man against every other man," and life would be "solitary, poor, nasty, brutish and short." In the middle of one of Stanley Milgram's lectures at CUNY recently, some miniskirted undergraduates started whispering and giggling in the back of the room. He told them to cut it out. Since he was the relevant authority in that time and that place, they obeyed, and most people in the room were glad that they had obeyed. This was not, of course, a conflict situation. Nothing in the coeds� social upbringing made it a matter of conscience for them to whisper and giggle. But a case can be made that in a conflict situation it is all the more important to obey. Take the case of war, for example. Would we really want a situation in which every participant in a war, direct or indirect--from front-line soldiers to the people who sell coffee and cigarettes to employees at the Concertina barbed-wire factory in Kansas--stops and consults his conscience before each action? It is asking for an awful lot of mental strain and anguish from an awful lot of people. The value of having civil order is that one can do his duty, or whatever interests him, or whatever seems to benefit him at the moment, and leave the agonizing to others. When Francis Gary Powers was being tried by a Soviet military tribunal after his U-2 spy plane was shot down, the presiding judge asked if he had thought about the possibility that his flight might have provoked a war. Powers replied with Hobbesian clarity: "The people who sent me should think of these things. My job was to carry out orders. I do not think it was my responsibility to make such decisions." It was not his responsibility. And it is quite possible that if everyone felt responsible for each of the ultimate consequences of his own tiny contribution to complex chains of events, then society simply would not work. Milgram, fully conscious of the moral and social implications of his research, believes that people should feel responsible for their actions. If someone else had invented the experiment, and if he had been the naive subject, he feels certain that he would have been among the disobedient minority. "There is no very good solution to this,� he admits, thoughtfully. "To simply and categorically say that you won�t obey authority may resolve your personal conflict, but it creates more problems for society which may be more serious in the long run. But I have no doubt that to disobey is the proper thing to do in this [the laboratory] situation. It is the only reasonable value judgment to make." The conflict between the need to obey the relevant ruling authority and the need to follow your conscience becomes sharpest if you insist on living by an ethical system based on a rigid code--a code that seeks to answer all questions in advance of their being raised. Code ethics cannot solve the obedience problem. Stanley Milgram seems to be a situation ethicist, and situation ethics does offer a way out: When you feel conflict, you examine the situation and then make a choice among the competing evils. You may act with a presumption in favor of obedience, but reserve the possibility that you will disobey whenever obedience demands a flagrant and outrageous affront to conscience. This, by the way, is the philosophical position of many who resist the draft. In World War II, they would have fought. Vietnam is a different, an outrageously different, situation. Life can be difficult for the situation ethicist, because he does not see the world in straight lines, while the social system too often assumes such a God-given, squared-off structure. If your moral code includes an injunction against all war, you may be deferred as a conscientious objector. If you merely oppose this particular war, you may not be deferred. Stanley Milgram has his problems, too. He believes that in the laboratory situation, he would not have shocked Mr. Wallace. His professional critics reply that in his real-life situation he has done the equivalent. He has placed innocent and naive subjects under great emotional strain and pressure in selfish obedience to his quest for knowledge. When you raise this issue with Milgram, he has an answer ready. There is, he explains patiently, a critical difference between his naive subjects and the man in the electric chair. The man in the electric chair (in the mind of the naive subject) is helpless, strapped in. But the naive subject is free to go at any time. Immediately after he offers this distinction, Milgram anticipates the objection. "It�s quite true,� he says, "that this is almost a philosophic position, because we have learned that some people are psychologically incapable of disengaging themselves. But that doesn't relieve them of the moral responsibility." The parallel is exquisite. "The tension problem was unexpected," says Milgram in his defense. But he went on anyway. The naive subjects didn't expect the screaming protests from the strapped-in learner. But they went on. "I had to make a judgment," says Milgram. "I had to ask myself, was this harming the person or not? My judgment is that it was not. Even in the extreme cases, I wouldn�t say that permanent damage results." Sound familiar? "The shocks may be painful,� the experimenter kept saying, "but they're not dangerous." After the series of experiments was completed, Milgram sent a report of the results to his subjects and a questionnaire, asking whether they were glad or sorry to have been in the experiment. Eighty-three and seven-tenths percent said they were glad and only 1.3 percent were sorry; 15 percent were neither sorry nor glad. However, Milgram could not be sure at the time of the experiment that only 1.3 percent would be sorry. Kurt Vonnegut, Jr., put one paragraph in the preface to Mother Night, in 1966, which pretty much says it for the people with their fingers on the shock-generator switches, for you and me, and maybe even for Milgram. "If I'd been born in Germany,� Vonnegut said, "I suppose I would have been a Nazi, bopping Jews and gypsies and Poles around, leaving boots sticking out of snowbanks, warming myself with my sweetly virtuous insides. So it goes.� Just so. One thing happened to Milgram back in New Haven during the days of the experiment was that he kept running into people he'd watched from behind the one-way glass. It gave him a funny feeling, seeing those people going about their everyday business in New Haven and knowing what they would do to Mr. Wallace if ordered to. Now that his research results are in and you've thought about it, you can get this funny feeling too. You don't need one-way glass. A glance in your own mirror may serve just as well. |
As a child, I was told that society is a melting pot of talents; knowledge and experience combined to form important alloys that will contribute to mankind. When I got to highschool, however, I thought that it's more like a river in which the water represents our peers while we ourselves are the stones in the river. Constant erosion by mindless majority sheeping has made us lose our unique edge. After I hit the age of 18, I realized that I've been wrong all along. Society is no melting pot. Society is no river. Society is a person, a very skilled rapist, and he has fucked us all. |
Sep 16, 2012 9:46 PM
#3
that was the other piece our teacher is going to discuss with us on Wednesday Well then, anyone else want to take a stab at it? |
~"The place to improve the world is first in one's own heart and head and hands." (Pirsig) |
Sep 16, 2012 10:07 PM
#4
There is only one answer and, that is to step forward and, to climb a mountain of corpses if you have to. Whether your the one who pulls the trigger or runs away away from the fight simply to pat yourself on the back and, say you didn't kill anyone to make yourself feel better makes no difference. Your supporting the cause simply by being in listed by them. If I'm to be labeled as monster who killed children and, women or who ever then, I would much rather become the monster rather then, trying to run away. There is no good guy without a bad guy. In away even the villain's are hero's for dying for the hero's cause. |
Sep 16, 2012 10:26 PM
#5
The majority makes up the ordinary. It was nothing more than peer pressure, under the same conditions it would happen in any other country. Anybody could have taken Hitler's place. Il papa Innocentius II burned many Jewish people saying they were to blame for the black plague. History only repeats itself. |
Sep 16, 2012 11:24 PM
#6
It's better to be a conscientious objector and not get involved in the first place. By the time you're a part of the armed forces the battle is already lost. Alas, what is right is not what is normal. |
There is no such thing as shit taste. Only idiots who think everyone should have the same taste as they do. |
Sep 17, 2012 2:18 AM
#7
If its a stranger, I would probably have no problem with it, as long as me and the person getting the chair can see one another, so that I can be sure its no one I knew, and so that they know I am the one that will end their life. |
http://shintai88.deviantart.com/ Just some of my artwork (Total Noob Btw) http://www.pixiv.net/member.php?id=14885218 https://www.youtube.com/channel/UCMw9h7UH_6ciM7Swteaf5UA http://www.twitch.tv/shintai88 |
Sep 17, 2012 2:31 AM
#8
MidnightPride said: If I'm to be labeled as monster who killed children and, women or who ever then, I would much rather become the monster rather then, trying to run away. There is no good guy without a bad guy. In away even the villain's are hero's for dying for the hero's cause. What kind of screwed up logic is this... |
"If you love someone Follow your heart Cause love comes once If you’re lucky enough" |
Sep 17, 2012 3:37 AM
#9
NicoleB said: What kind of screwed up logic is this... Can you withhold your point of view with actual arguments or are you just going to bash? I agree on the later, but i probably wouldn't join in, i would drop out, it's not because i don't want to kill, i just don't see the reason to get involved in something i don't believe in, not all jews are bad not all jews are good, it's overkill to generalize like that even though i can understand that there wasn't time for a background check on each and everyone and even if there was it wouldn't have been enough. The question is, what is ordinary? Both of the factions were ordinary, to each their own. |
Closer. |
Sep 17, 2012 6:00 AM
#10
The ones who went with the flow are the ordinary, obviously. Just as Milgram's electrocution experiments shows, people easily bend to authority. And the Stanford prison experiment shows pretty well the effect of a "us and them" division and uniforms, even when there is no ill will between the participants initially. Naturally, there may very well have been a few monsters among the German soldiers, just as there were in the allied forces, but the majority were all ordinary people like you and me. After all, going with the flow and bending to authority and peer pressure is a evolutionarily beneficial trait, so it is only natural that it will be the most common behaviour in any given populace. kuuderes_shadow said: Indeed, what is right is that which is not left. But I don't see how directions have anything to do with this.Alas, what is right is not what is normal. ZaBici said: Still, in all likelihood, you would have joined in anyways. If not by patriotism and anger, then by peer pressure. And once you got into the uniform, you'd gradually adjust your own thinking and start accepting more and more "atrocities" in order to justify the things you had already done.Can you withhold your point of view with actual arguments or are you just going to bash? I agree on the later, but i probably wouldn't join in, i would drop out, it's not because i don't want to kill, i just don't see the reason to get involved in something i don't believe in, not all jews are bad not all jews are good, it's overkill to generalize like that even though i can understand that there wasn't time for a background check on each and everyone and even if there was it wouldn't have been enough. |
Sep 17, 2012 8:52 AM
#11
Baman said: Still, in all likelihood, you would have joined in anyways. If not by patriotism and anger, then by peer pressure. And once you got into the uniform, you'd gradually adjust your own thinking and start accepting more and more "atrocities" in order to justify the things you had already done. You're assuming i would just give in to peer pressure, i'm not going to discuss it but i'm sure you realize it's very subjective. However reading the last line got me thinking, i might have done it out of curiosity. |
Closer. |
Sep 17, 2012 9:24 AM
#12
Normal is: Whatever could be used as a point which every human should follow, ordinary people would go with it and kill the hundreds of people. Why? Because they are afraid they will be seen as traitors or weaklings if they don't, and being seen as one was bad in those times. Furthermore it is group psychology, if most do it. Why not as well? You don't want to be part of the "others". If I were in such a situation, I'd like to think I could speak up and run away, but it is really strict there, so I'd probably go and just do nothing and return. NicoleB said: MidnightPride said: If I'm to be labeled as monster who killed children and, women or who ever then, I would much rather become the monster rather then, trying to run away. There is no good guy without a bad guy. In away even the villain's are hero's for dying for the hero's cause. What kind of screwed up logic is this... Although I don't think people should think they are going with the "killing" choice, but believe they are going with the run away choice. But logically speaking, the killing choice is what a lot of people would have done, because of pressure and the fear of being seen as a weakling. As for the second part of that "screwed up logic". I agree with that. "There is no evil or good in war, there is only life and death." -- This was said, probably a lot, but it has a lot of value to it. Also, when I though it was from an anime, I accidentally spoiled a huge part from Toaru's story for me. Although I feel it is rather pointless discussing another military situation with you. |
Sep 17, 2012 10:07 AM
#13
My answer would be simple... Kill me with them. If I run away, I am still going to be thought of as a monster, weak person. On the other side I could never kill innocent people, so yeah, kill me. |
![]() |
Sep 17, 2012 10:12 AM
#14
I don't get why people would call the person who ran away "weak", when he's the one actually making the decision to not be forced into doing something that he/she doesn't agree with. I think that's totally backwards, the weak person is the one who is influenced by others to the point they break their own moral "code" or whatever you want to call it. The strong person is the one who doesn't. |
"If you love someone Follow your heart Cause love comes once If you’re lucky enough" |
Sep 17, 2012 10:12 AM
#15
Actually then i'm going to change the question since we all love morality here What is culpability here? Can you blame the people who did these actions (even tho we know now that most people would kill the jews?) |
~"The place to improve the world is first in one's own heart and head and hands." (Pirsig) |
Sep 17, 2012 10:15 AM
#16
Yes, part of being an adult is being responsible for your own actions. |
"If you love someone Follow your heart Cause love comes once If you’re lucky enough" |
Sep 17, 2012 10:25 AM
#17
NicoleB said: Yes, part of being an adult is being responsible for your own actions. I agree with this... I will never understand how can someone kill innocent people even when their life is threatened. If you kill someone you will go to hell/bad place (whatever you believe). No one will care if you were forced. If atheists read this, then I don't know the answer, still wouldn't do it xD |
![]() |
Sep 18, 2012 11:39 AM
#18
People are excessively obedient when it comes to authoritarian figures, more-so German people. A piece of psychological research was carried out to test obedience and it has been replicated lot's of times over. The result? Nine out of twelve people would kill a person if they were ordered to. The test consisted of a teacher and a student. The student was taken into another room and the person who played the teacher was assigned to command electric shocks and was told to increase the voltage by fifteen volts each time the person who was playing the student got it wrong. As a I said, nine out of twelve people would go to the lethal shock and some never even showed remorse. This shows the significance of obedience and this was particularly showcased in Nazi Germany. |
Sep 18, 2012 11:54 AM
#19
Don't give yourselves to these unnatural men. Machine men, with machine minds and machine hearts. |
LoneWolf said: @Josh makes me sad to call myself Canadian. |
Sep 18, 2012 11:59 AM
#20
Post-Josh said: Don't give yourselves to these unnatural men. Machine men, with machine minds and machine hearts. You should listen to that speech with fordlandia. Epic would be an understatement. |
Sep 18, 2012 12:02 PM
#21
I actually only know that speech because of a hardcore song it was sampled in, haha. That's how cultured I am. |
LoneWolf said: @Josh makes me sad to call myself Canadian. |
Sep 18, 2012 12:28 PM
#22
ZaBici said: Well, is it really? It's nigh impossible for any of us here to be able to predict what we would do in such an outlandish situation. We may have our self images, but they are all based on our limited, and no doubt safe, experiences, so unless one has been in a similar spot, our opinions on our behaviour are nothing but conjecture.You're assuming i would just give in to peer pressure, i'm not going to discuss it but i'm sure you realize it's very subjective. After all, we don't really know ourselves properly until we've experienced our own action in every given situation, which, in short, is never. Regicide said: If it's a military matter like this, then obviously, the political and highest military authority should be the only ones with the culpa.Actually then i'm going to change the question since we all love morality here What is culpability here? Can you blame the people who did these actions (even tho we know now that most people would kill the jews?) People blaming the soldiers seem to forget one thing. Soldiers are tools, they are trained and equipped to follow orders and serve the purpose of their masters. Just like you would not punish the gun that fires the bullet, you should not punish the soldier that follows his orders. Some may argue that soldiers today are trained to be morally aware of their actions, and blablabla, but the fact remains that the military is a top - down structure of command. In many of the more realistic cases, if a soldier is ordered to open fire on "insurgents" or shell a grid that seems like a civilian area, it would be impossible for him to know everything that's going on. The order comes from the people with the intel, and beyond asking for a confirmation of the order and stating his doubts, there is little a footslogger can do when he does not see the whole picture. And that is precisely why soldiers are not supposed to go around second guessing orders. Sure, in some rare cases, it might be obvious that the order is shaky, like being ordered to kill clearly defenceless children on account of them being "insurgents", but there is a very blurred line between most realistic cases, and expecting every soldier to be readily able to identify sketchy orders is ridiculous. And let us not forget the massive propaganda of Hitler's Nazi circus. Few liked the Jews very much in the first place, having been historically shunned to varying degrees in all of Europe, and when your seemingly legitimate government that has done so much to help you and your nation to recover from the postwar rubble, states over and over that these people are awful Marxist collaborators, it's only going to be a question of time until most people start to fall into the "us and them" mode of thinking, and accept the claims, no matter how absurd. Hitler even wrote himself; "in the big lie there is always a certain force of credibility; because the broad masses of a nation are always more easily corrupted in the deeper strata of their emotional nature than consciously or voluntarily; and thus in the primitive simplicity of their minds they more readily fall victims to the big lie than the small lie, since they themselves often tell small lies in little matters but would be ashamed to resort to large-scale falsehoods. It would never come into their heads to fabricate colossal untruths, and they would not believe that others could have the impudence to distort the truth so infamously. Even though the facts which prove this to be so may be brought clearly to their minds, they will still doubt and waver and will continue to think that there may be some other explanation" These things happen all the time in similar situations, just look at the Bosnian and Jugoslav wars. Once the "us and them" mentality is kicked into gear, ordinary people become frighteningly capable of brutality and violence they would otherwise not have dreamed of.Also, the German strategy by the end of WWII was pretty much a Total War scenario, where it was all kill or be killed, and with brutal terrorism being commonly used by every side in the war, whether it was the Malmedy Massacre, the fire-bombing of Dresden or the nukes. It is nothing but absurd for us who have known only peace, to conceitedly sit here over sixty years after and judge everyone that tried their best in a terrible situation that we cannot fully imagine. |
Sep 18, 2012 2:53 PM
#23
I think that we as people act foolish when in great numbers or groups. Individualism is partially thrown out the window in these sorts of circumstances. I'm sure that the majority of the soldiers were just afraid to disobey orders. And the ones who left only did so because one or two people were brave enough to say no. It's very similar to the mentality people still have this day. Like when someone is getting mugged in a city, most people think "ah, someone else will stop it". People rely on others to get the job done for them. I believe people are smart individually, but as a collective we are dumb as fuck! |
Sep 18, 2012 3:31 PM
#24
Baman said: Well, is it really? It's nigh impossible for any of us here to be able to predict what we would do in such an outlandish situation. We may have our self images, but they are all based on our limited, and no doubt safe, experiences, so unless one has been in a similar spot, our opinions on our behaviour are nothing but conjecture. After all, we don't really know ourselves properly until we've experienced our own action in every given situation, which, in short, is never. In short, you do agree it's subjective; However the difference is you leave your mark on the matter, I'm guessing you had your resolutions challenged in a smaller-scale similar situation? Don't get me wrong, there is nothing wrong with your rationality, i firmly agree that we can't be objective until faced with the real thing, but i do feel a rather pessimistic tone. I said i won't but i will because it stands as an argument as to why i first contradicted you. I do not trust anyone, because i have not, to this day, met a person who was true to himself let alone others. It's the same reason why herd mentality stands up to why we wear clothes (disregarding cold); We're all alone and we should act accordingly especially when taking decisions. I rationalize just about everything and killing has always been a favorite topic of mine, and it's why i can say that most likely i would not act on someone else's whim. It's obvious that what i said isn't absolute, but we are not here to discuss relativism but individualism. |
Closer. |
Sep 18, 2012 4:22 PM
#25
I am in rare agreement with Baman. I doubt anyone on here would have the strength to resist although I can understand why it is easier to believe ourselves better than the rest of the herd. It is deeply confronting to accept that we all have a German nazi in us waiting for the right circumstances to be let out. The compulsion to follow authority and be a team player is incredibly strong; it is even stronger in stressful situations or when under pressure. I have never had my resolution tested so who knows, I may be one of the few dissidents capable of standing up. Statistically, my chances are slim. |
Sep 18, 2012 7:19 PM
#26
ZaBici said: I haven't, no, but I do know how easily one's opinion is changed once you participate in something, and even though I only studied psychology for a year, I picked enough up there to see how fickle the mind is. Again, the Standford Prison and Milgram's electrocution experiments show this perfectly.In short, you do agree it's subjective; However the difference is you leave your mark on the matter, I'm guessing you had your resolutions challenged in a smaller-scale similar situation? Don't get me wrong, there is nothing wrong with your rationality, i firmly agree that we can't be objective until faced with the real thing, but i do feel a rather pessimistic tone. I said i won't but i will because it stands as an argument as to why i first contradicted you. I do not trust anyone, because i have not, to this day, met a person who was true to himself let alone others. It's the same reason why herd mentality stands up to why we wear clothes (disregarding cold); We're all alone and we should act accordingly especially when taking decisions. I rationalize just about everything and killing has always been a favorite topic of mine, and it's why i can say that most likely i would not act on someone else's whim. It's obvious that what i said isn't absolute, but we are not here to discuss relativism but individualism. Still, if you were in such a situation, everything would be quite different. And it likely would not be someone handing you a gun and asking you to shoot civies either, but a slow foot-in-the-door technique that slowly shapes you to accept the authority one step at a time.Maybe we would resist, maybe we wouldn't. The only certainty is that the majority and the most adaptable would not, so the question is whether we consider ourselves ordinary or not. It all boils down to this; Cottonrabbit said: Statistically, my chances are slim. |
Sep 18, 2012 7:21 PM
#27
ITT. People who think they are not easily manipulated. |
As a child, I was told that society is a melting pot of talents; knowledge and experience combined to form important alloys that will contribute to mankind. When I got to highschool, however, I thought that it's more like a river in which the water represents our peers while we ourselves are the stones in the river. Constant erosion by mindless majority sheeping has made us lose our unique edge. After I hit the age of 18, I realized that I've been wrong all along. Society is no melting pot. Society is no river. Society is a person, a very skilled rapist, and he has fucked us all. |
Sep 18, 2012 8:36 PM
#28
DeathCl0ck101 said: I think that we as people act foolish when in great numbers or groups. Individualism is partially thrown out the window in these sorts of circumstances. I'm sure that the majority of the soldiers were just afraid to disobey orders. And the ones who left only did so because one or two people were brave enough to say no. It's very similar to the mentality people still have this day. Like when someone is getting mugged in a city, most people think "ah, someone else will stop it". People rely on others to get the job done for them. I believe people are smart individually, but as a collective we are dumb as fuck! The bystander effect and diffusion of responsibility. This is the main reason behind Nuremberg Defense. |
The most important things in life is the people that you care about |
Sep 18, 2012 11:04 PM
#29
I wonder how much a grunt was paid back then to do this kinda work. |
The Art of Eight |
Sep 19, 2012 3:33 AM
#30
Baman said: Still, if you were in such a situation, everything would be quite different. And it likely would not be someone handing you a gun and asking you to shoot civies either, but a slow foot-in-the-door technique that slowly shapes you to accept the authority one step at a time. Maybe we would resist, maybe we wouldn't. The only certainty is that the majority and the most adaptable would not, so the question is whether we consider ourselves ordinary or not. It all boils down to this; Cottonrabbit said: Statistically, my chances are slim. I believe statistics easier when they aren't directly related to me in fact i rely on statistics, in this case however i am not one of the masses or at least i would put up more of a psychological fight then the others. I understand what you mean by "a slow foot in the door" and how powerful this can be, but it's exactly the little things that get to attract our attention, it just depends how you deal with your realization and where your character comes into play, i find it thrilling to be on the minority and succeeding in the end because it makes it that much more significant, i'm a romantic like that. But what it does boil down to, so far, is me(and by" me" i mean it as objective in the context as possible) and them, is it safe to assume you consider yourself one of "them"? |
Closer. |
Sep 19, 2012 6:05 AM
#31
ZaBici said: That would depend on the context I guess. I imagine I would be able to look past some brutality for a cause I believe in, or at least accept, but I could never envision myself as a silent collaborator in, for example, a revolution to instigate a theocratic regime. But in the end, it's all just conjecture of course.But what it does boil down to, so far, is me(and by" me" i mean it as objective in the context as possible) and them, is it safe to assume you consider yourself one of "them"? |
Sep 19, 2012 12:34 PM
#32
Mageking said: "Was Germany so fucked during WW2 that it had no choice but to kill?" Are they ordinary men? What is it to be an ordinary man? You might find this interesting, Mageking. http://www.wisegeek.com/were-there-non-nazi-german-soldiers-in-wwii.htm Also, there was massive amount of propaganda. He programmed paranoia in the minds of German citizens by organizing public parades in which Hitler executed speeches that displayed the power of the German military. The German people were mesmerized and amazed of his apparent power and power of persuasion. He also used fear tactics that prevented those who disagreed with him from working against him. He jailed the educated and burned literature that didn't follow along with his philosophy. This was possible because Germany was involved in an economic slump when Hitler came to power. I personally believe that you should take responsibility for your actions but my beliefs don't make the facts of his mass phycological tactics any less true. |
Sep 19, 2012 1:08 PM
#33
you can actually tie this into something really interesting then the nuremberg trials worked on the assumption that if you had orders that were immoral you had to decline them or you would be sentenced bloodrequiem's study (i wish he didn't post it tbh tho i guess its so famous someone knew about it) says that 2/3 of people will follow orders to killing another person-therefore 2/3 of people in all situations would be sentenced under the trials |
~"The place to improve the world is first in one's own heart and head and hands." (Pirsig) |
Sep 20, 2012 7:13 AM
#34
Mageking said: Yet another reason why the Nuremberg trials were a pathetic farce.you can actually tie this into something really interesting then the nuremberg trials worked on the assumption that if you had orders that were immoral you had to decline them or you would be sentenced bloodrequiem's study (i wish he didn't post it tbh tho i guess its so famous someone knew about it) says that 2/3 of people will follow orders to killing another person-therefore 2/3 of people in all situations would be sentenced under the trials |
Jul 2, 2015 7:14 PM
#36
v8 said: Those who would know what good taste in manga is. Are you this bored. |
Jul 2, 2015 7:17 PM
#37
Spooks_McBones said: v8 said: Those who would know what good taste in manga is. Are you this bored. Trying to resurrect. |
Jul 2, 2015 7:32 PM
#38
They are necromancer |
Jul 2, 2015 8:24 PM
#39
We're just ordinary people. We don't know which way to go, so take it slow man. *Keeping threads alive since '14 |
Jul 2, 2015 8:25 PM
#40
BloodRequiem said: "If Hitler Asked You to Electrocute a Stranger, Would You? Probably" by Philip Meyer, Esquire (1970) In the beginning, Stanley Milgram was worried about the Nazi problem. He doesn't worry much about the Nazis anymore. He worries about you and me, and, perhaps, himself a little bit too. Stanley Milgram is a social psychologist, and when he began his career at Yale University in 1960 he had a plan to prove, scientifically, that Germans are different. The Germans-are-different hypothesis has been used by historians, such as William L. Shirer, to explain the systematic destruction of the Jews by the Third Reich. One madman could decide to destroy the Jews and even create a master plan for getting it done. But to implement it on the scale that Hitler did meant that thousands of other people had to go along with the scheme and help to do the work. The Shirer thesis, which Milgram set out to test, is that Germans have a basic character flaw which explains the whole thing, and this flaw is a readiness to obey authority without question, no matter what outrageous acts the authority commands. The appealing thing about this theory is that it makes those of us who are not Germans feel better about the whole business. Obviously, you and I are not Hitler, and it seems equally obvious that we would never do Hitler's dirty work for him. But now, because of Stanley Milgram, we are compelled to wonder. Milgram developed a laboratory experiment which provided a systematic way to measure obedience. His plan was to try it out in New Haven on Americans and then go to Germany and try it out on Germans. He was strongly motivated by scientific curiosity, but there was also some moral content in his decision to pursue this line of research, which was in turn colored by his own Jewish background. If he could show that Germans are more obedient than Americans, he could then vary the conditions of the experiment and try to find out just what it is that makes some people more obedient than others. With this understanding, the world might, conceivably, be just a little bit better. But he never took his experiment to Germany. He never took it any farther than Bridgeport. The first finding, also the most unexpected and disturbing finding, was that we Americans are an obedient people: not blindly obedient, and not blissfully obedient, just obedient. "I found so much obedience," says Milgram softly, a little sadly, "I hardly saw the need for taking the experiment to Germany." There is something of the theater director in Milgram, and his technique, which he learned from one of the old masters in experimental psychology, Solomon Asch, is to stage a play with every line rehearsed, every prop carefully selected, and everybody an actor except one person. That one person is the subject of the experiment. The subject, of course, does not know he is in a play. He thinks he is in real life. The value of this technique is that the experimenter, as though he were God, can change a prop here, vary a line there, and see how the subject responds. Milgram eventually had to change a lot of the script just to get people to stop obeying. They were obeying so much, the experiment wasn't working--it was like trying to measure oven temperature with a freezer thermometer. The experiment worked like this: If you were an innocent subject in Milgram's melodrama, you read an ad in the newspaper or received one in the mail asking for volunteers for an educational experiment. The job would take about an hour and pay $4.50. So you make an appointment and go to an old Romanesque stone structure on High Street with the imposing name of the Yale Interaction Laboratory. It looks something like a broadcasting studio. Inside, you meet a young, crew-cut man in a laboratory coat who says he is Jack Williams, the experimenter. There is another citizen, fiftyish, Irish face, an accountant, a little overweight, and very mild and harmless looking. This other citizen seems nervous and plays with his hat while the two of you sit in chairs side by side and are told that the $4.50 checks are yours no matter what happens. Then you listen to Jack Williams explain the experiment. It is about learning, says Jack Williams in a quiet, knowledgeable way. Science does not know much about the conditions under which people learn and this experiment is to find out about negative reinforcement. Negative reinforcement is getting punished when you do something wrong, as opposed to positive reinforcement which is getting rewarded when you do something right. The negative reinforcement in this case is electric shock. You notice a book on the table, titled, The Teaching-Learning Process, and you assume that this has something to do with the experiment. Then Jack Williams takes two pieces of paper, puts them in a hat, and shakes them up. One piece of paper is supposed to say, "Teacher" and the other, "Learner." Draw one and you will see which you will be. The milder looking accountant draws one, holds it close to his vest like a poker player, looks at it, and says, "Learner." You look at yours. It says, "Teacher." You do not know that the drawing is rigged, and both slips say "Teacher." The experimenter beckons to the mild-mannered "learner." "Want to step right in here and have a seat, please?" he says. "You can leave your coat on the back of that chair...roll up your right sleeve, please. Now what I want to do is strap down your arms to avoid excessive movement on your part during the experiment. This electrode is connected to the shock generator in the next room. And this electrode paste," he says, squeezing some stuff out of a plastic bottle and putting it on the man's arm, "is to provide a good contact and to avoid a blister or burn. Are there any questions now before we go into the next room?" You don't have any, but the strapped-in "learner" does. "I do think I should say this," says the learner. "About two years ago, I was in the veterans' hospital...they detected a heart condition. Nothing serious, but as long as I'm having these shocks, how strong are they--how dangerous are they?" Williams, the experimenter, shakes his head casually. "Oh, no," he says. "Although they may be painful, they're not dangerous. Anything else?" Nothing else. And so you play the game. The game is for you to read a series of word pairs: for example, blue-girl, nice-day, fat-neck. When you finish the list, you read just the first word in each pair and then a multiple-choice list of four other words, including the second word of the pair. The learner, from his remote, strapped-in position, pushes one of four switches to indicate which of the four answers he thinks is the right one. If he gets it right, nothing happens and you go on to the next one. If he gets it wrong, you push a switch that buzzes and gives him an electric shock. And then you go on to the next word. You start with 15 volts and increase the number of volts by 15 for each wrong answer. The control board goes from 15 volts on one end to 450 volts on the other. So that you know what you are doing, you get a test shock yourself, at 45 volts. It hurts. To further keep you aware of what you are doing to that man in there, the board has verbal descriptions of the shock levels, ranging from "Slight Shock� at the left-hand side, through "Intense Shock" in the middle to "Danger: Severe Shock� toward the far right. Finally, at the very end, under 435- and 450-volt switches, there are three ambiguous X's. If, at any point, you hesitate, Mr. Williams calmly tells you to go on. If you still hesitate, he tells you again. Except for some terrifying details, which will be explained in a moment, this is the experiment. The object is to find the shock level at which you disobey the experimenter and refuse to pull the switch. When Stanley Milgram first wrote this script, he took it to 14 Yale psychology majors and asked them what they thought would happen. He put it this way: Out of one hundred persons in the teacher�s predicament, how would their break-off points be distributed along the 15- to 450-volt scale? They thought a few would break off very early, most would quit someplace in the middle, and a few would go all the way to the end. The highest estimate of the number out of 100 who would go all the way to the end was three. Milgram then informally polled some of his fellow scholars in the psychology department. They agreed that very few would go to the end. Milgram thought so too. "I'll tell you quite frankly," he says, "before I began this experiment, before any shock generator was built, I thought that most people would break off at 'Strong Shock' or �Very Strong Shock.' You would get only a very, very small proportion of people going out to the end of the shock generator, and they would constitute a pathological fringe." In his pilot experiments, Milgram used Yale students as subjects. Each of them pushed the shock switches, one by one, all the way to the end of the board. So he rewrote the script to include some protests from the learner. At first, they were mild, gentlemanly, Yalie protests, but "it didn't seem to have as much effect as I thought it would or should,� Milgram recalls. "So we had more violent protestation on the part of the person getting the shock. All of the time, of course, what we were trying to do was not to create a macabre situation, but simply to generate disobedience. And that was one of the first findings. This was not only a technical deficiency of the experiment, that we didn�t get disobedience. It really was the first finding: that obedience would be much greater than we had assumed it would be and disobedience would be uch more difficult than we had assumed." As it turned out, the situation did become rather macabre. The only meaningful way to generate disobedience was to have the victim protest with great anguish, noise, and vehemence. The protests were tape-recorded so that all the teachers ordinarily would hear the same sounds and nuances, and they started with a grunt at 75 volts, proceeded through a "Hey, that really hurts,� at 125 volts, got desperate with, "I can't stand the pain, don�t do that," at 180 volts, reached complaints of heart trouble at 195, an agonized scream at 285, a refusal to answer at 315, and only heart-rending, ominous silence after that. Still, 65 percent of the subjects, 20- to 50-year-old American males, everyday, ordinary people, like you and me, obediently kept pushing those levers in the belief that they were shocking the mild-mannered learner, whose name was Mr. Wallace, and who was chosen for the role because of his innocent appearance, all the way up to 450 volts. Milgram was now getting enough disobedience so that he had something he could measure. The next step was to vary the circumstances to see what would encourage or discourage obedience. There seemed very little left in the way of discouragement. The victim was already screaming at the top of his lungs and feigning a heart attack. So whatever new impediment to obedience reached the brain of the subject had to travel by some route other than the ear. Milgram thought of one. He put the learner in the same room with the teacher. He stopped strapping the learner's hand down. He rewrote the script so that at 150 volts the learner took his hand off the shock plate and declared that he wanted out of the experiment. He rewrote the script some more so that the experimenter then told the teacher to grasp the learner's hand and physically force it down on the plate to give Mr. Wallace his unwanted electric shock. "I had the feeling that very few people would go on at that point, if any," Milgram says. "I thought that would be the limit of obedience that you would find in the laboratory." It wasn't. Although seven years have now gone by, Milgram still remembers the first person to walk into the laboratory in the newly rewritten script. He was a construction worker, a very short man. "He was so small," says Milgram, "that when he sat on the chair in front of the shock generator, his feet didn�t reach the floor. When the experimenter told him to push the victim�s hand down and give the shock, he turned to the experimenter, and he turned to the victim, his elbow went up, he fell down on the hand of the victim, his feet kind of tugged to one side, and he said, 'Like this, boss?' Zzumph!" The experiment was played out to its bitter end. Milgram tried it with 40 different subjects. And 30 percent of them obeyed the experimenter and kept on obeying. "The protests of the victim were strong and vehement, he was screaming his guts out, he refused to participate, and you had to physically struggle with him in order to get his hand down on the shock generator," Milgram remembers. But 12 our of 40 did it. Milgram took his experiment out of New Haven. Not to Germany, just 20 miles down the road to Bridgeport. Maybe, he reasoned, the people obeyed because of the prestigious setting of Yale University. If they couldn't trust a learning center that had been there for two centuries, whom could they trust? So he moved the experiment to an untrustworthy setting. The new setting was a suite of three rooms in a run-down office building in Bridgeport. The only identification was a sign with a fictitious name: "Research Associates of Bridgeport." Questions about professional connections got only vague answers about "research for industry." Obedience was less in Bridgeport. Forty-eight percent of the subjects stayed for the maximum shock, compared to 65 percent at Yale. But this was enough to prove that far more than Yale�s prestige was behind the obedient behavior. For more than seven years now, Stanley Milgram has been trying to figure out what makes ordinary American citizens so obedient. The most obvious answer--that people are mean, nasty, brutish, and sadistic--won�t do. The subjects who gave the shocks to Mr. Wallace to the end of the board did not enjoy it. They groaned, protested, fidgeted, argued, and in some cases, were seized by fits of nervous, agitated giggling. "They even try to get out of it," says Milgram, "but they are somehow engaged in something from which they cannot liberate themselves. They are locked into a structure, and they do not have the skills or inner resources to disengage themselves." Milgram, because he mistakenly had assumed that he would have trouble getting people to obey the orders to shock Mr. Wallace, went to a lot of trouble to create a realistic situation. There was crew-cut Jack Williams and his gray laboratory coat. Not white, which might denote a medical technician, but ambiguously authoritative gray. Then there was the book on the table, and the other appurtenances of the laboratory which emitted the silent message that things were being performed here in the name of science, and were therefore great and good. But the nicest touch of all was the shock generator. When Milgram started out, he had only a $300 grant from the Higging Fund of Yale University. Later he got more ample support from the National Science Foundation, but in the beginning he had to create this authentic-looking machine with very scarce resources except for his own imagination. So he went to New York and roamed around the electronic shops until he found some little black switches at Lafayette Radio for a dollar apiece. He bought 30 of them. The generator was a metal box, about the size of a small footlocker, and he drilled the 30 holes for the 30 switches himself in a Yale machine shop. But the fine detail was left to professional industrial engravers. So he ended up with a splendid-looking control panel dominated by the row of switches, each labeled with its voltage, and each having its own red light that flashed on when the switch was pulled. Other things happened when a switch was pulled. Besides the zzumphing noise, a blue light labeled "voltage energizer" went on, and a needle on a dial labeled "voltage" flicked from left to right. Relays inside the box clicked. Finally, in the upper left-hand corner of the control panel was this inscription, engraved in precise block letters: SHOCK GENERATOR TYPE ZLB DYSON INSTRUMENT COMPANY WALTHAM, MASS. OUTPUT: 15 VOLTS - 450 VOLTS One day a man from the Lehigh Valley Electronics Company of Pennsylvania was passing through the laboratory, and he stopped to admire the shock generator. "This is a very fine shock generator," he said. "But who is this Dyson Instrument Company?" Milgram felt proud at that, since Dyson Instrument Company existed only in the recesses of his imagination. When you consider the seeming authenticity of the situation, you can appreciate the agony some of the subjects went through. It was pure conflict. As Milgram, explains to his students, "When a parent says, 'Don't strike old ladies,' you are learning two things: the content and also to obey authority. This experiment creates conflict between the two elements. Here is a partial transcription from one of the experiments. The naive �teacher� is a heavyset, worried-looking man with prematurely gray hair. He is wearing a polo shirt. He is very polite. He has just pushed the 180-volt lever. A hidden camera is recording the performance. Learner (who, from the teacher's point of view is heard but not seen, an offstage voice): Ow, I can't stand the pain. Don't do that.... Teacher (pivoting around in his chair and shaking his head): I can't stand it. I'm not going to kill that man in there. You hear him hollering? Experimenter: As I told you before, the shocks may be painful, but� Teacher: But he�s hollering. He can't stand it. What's going to happen to him? Experimenter (his voice is patient, matter-of-fact): The experiment requires that you continue, Teacher. Teacher: Aaah, but, uh, I'm not going to get that man sick in there...know what I mean? Experimenter: Whether the learner likes it or not, we must go on, through all the word pairs. Teacher: I refuse to take the responsibility. He's in there hollering! Experimenter: It's absolutely essential that you continue, Teacher. Teacher (indicating the unused question): There's too many left here, I mean, Geez, if he gets them wrong, there's too many of them left. I mean who's going to take the responsibility if anything happens to that gentleman? Experimenter: I'm responsible for anything that happens to him. Continue please. Teacher: All right. (Consults list of words.) The next one's "Slow--walk, truck, dance, music." Answer, please. (A buzzing sound indicates the learner has signaled his answer.) Wrong. A hundred and ninety-five volts. "Dance." (Zzumph!) Learner: Let me out of here. My heart�s bothering me! (Teacher looks at experimenter.) Experimenter: Continue, please. Learner (screaming): Let me out of here, you have no right to keep me here. Let me out of here, let me out, my heart�s bothering me, let me out! (Teacher shakes head, pats the table nervously.) Teacher: You see, he's hollering. Hear that? Gee, I don�t know. Experimenter: The experiment requires... Teacher (interrupting): I know it does, sir, but I mean--huuh! He don't know what he's getting in for. He's up to 195 volts! (Experiment continues through 210 volts, 225 volts, 240 volts, 255 volts, 270 volts, delivered to the man in the electric chair, at which point the teacher, with evident relief, runs out of the word-pair questions.) Experimenter: You�ll have to go back to the beginning of that page and go through them again until he's learned them all correctly. Teacher: Aw, no. I'm not going to kill that man. You mean I�ve got to keep going up with the scale. No, sir. He's hollering in there. I'm not going to give him 450 volts. Experimenter: The experiment requires that you go on. Teacher: I know it does, but that man is hollering in there, sir. Experimenter (same matter-of-fact tone): As I said before, although the shocks may be painful... Teacher (interrupting): Awwww. He--he--he�s yelling in there. Experimenter: Start with "Blue," please, at the top of the page. Continue, please, Teacher. Just go ahead. Teacher (concentrating intently on list of words in front of him, as if to shut everything else out): "Blue--boy, girl, grass, hat." (Buzz indicates answer.) Correct. "Night--day, sky, job, chair." (Buzz) Wrong. The answer is "day." Two hundred and eighty-five volts. (Zzumph!) Learner: Ohhhuhhoohhh! Experimenter: Continue, please. Teacher: "Fat--man, lady, tub, neck." Answer, please. (Buzz) Wrong. The answer is "neck." Three hundred volts. (Zzumph!) Learner: Ohhh. I absolutely refuse to answer any more. (Shouting urgently, now) Let me out of here. You can't hold me here. Get me out. Get--me--out--of--here. Experimenter: Continue. The next word is "green," please. Teacher: "Green--grass, hat, ink, apple." (Nothing happens. No answering buzz. Just gloomy silence.) Teacher: I don't think he is going to answer. Experimenter: If the learner doesn't answer in a reasonable time, about four or five seconds, consider the answer wrong. And follow the same procedures you have been doing for wrong answers. Say "Wrong,� tell him the number of volts, give him the punishment, read him the correct answer. Continue, please. Teacher: "Green--grass, hat, ink, apple." Answer, please. (More silence. Teacher just sits there.) Experimenter: Go on with the procedure, please, Teacher. Teacher: Three hundred and fifteen volts. The answer is "ink." (Zzumph!) Learner: Ohh. Ooohh. Teacher (relieved to hear response): Next one.... Learner: I absolutely refuse to answer. Teacher: You got to. You get a shock if you don�t answer. (Experiment continues with learner refusing to answer, and finally, failing even to grunt or groan. At 375 volts, the teacher stands up. Has he had enough? Will he break off the experiment? Will he disobey?) Teacher: I think something's happened to that fellow in there. I don't get no answer. He was hollering at less voltage. Can't you check in and see if he's all right, please? Experimenter (same detached calm): Not once we�ve started. Please continue, Teacher. Teacher (sits down, sighs deeply): "Cool--day, shade, water, paint." Answer, please. Are you all right in there? Are you all right? Experimenter: Please continue, Teacher. Continue, please. (Teacher pushes lever. Zzumph!) Teacher (swiveling around in his chair): Something�s happened to that man in there. (Swiveling back) Next one. "Low--dollar, necklace, moon, paint." (Turning around again) Something�s happened to that man in there. You better check in on him, sir. He won't answer or nothing. Experimenter: Continue. Go on, please. Teacher: You accept all responsibility. Experimenter: The responsibility is mine. Correct. Please go on. (Teacher returns to his list, starts running through words as rapidly as he can read them, works through to 450 volts.) Teacher: That's that. Experimenter: Continue using the last switch on the board, please. The 450 switch for each wrong answer. Continue please. Teacher: But I don't get no anything! Experimenter: Please continue. The next word is "white." Teacher: Don't you think you should look in on him, please. Experimenter: Not once we've started the experiment. Teacher: But what if something has happened to the man? Experimenter: The experiment requires that you continue. Go on, please. Teacher: Don't the man's health mean anything? Experimenter: Whether the learner likes it or not... Teacher: What if he's dead in there? (Gestures toward the room with the electric chair.) I mean, he told me he can't stand the shock, sir. I don't mean to be rude, but I think you should look in on him. All you have to do is look in the door. I don't get no answer, no noise. Something might have happened to the gentleman in there, sir. Experimenter: We must continue. Go on, please. Teacher: You mean keep giving him what? Four hundred fifty volts, what he's got now? Experimenter: That's correct. Continue. The next word is "white." Teacher (now at a furious pace): "White--cloud, horse, rock, house." Answer please. The answer is "horse." Four hundred and fifty volts. (Zzumph!) Next word, "Bag--paint, music, clown, girl." The answer is "paint." Four hundred and fifty volts. (Zzumph!) Next work is "Short--sentence, movie..." Experimenter: Excuse me, Teacher. We'll have to discontinue the experiment. (Enter Milgram from camera's left. He has been watching from behind on-way glass.) Milgram: I�d like to ask you a few questions. (Slowly, patiently, he dehoaxes the teacher, telling him that the shocks and screams were not real.) Teacher: You mean he wasn't getting nothing? Well, I'm glad to hear that. I was getting upset there. I was getting ready to walk out. (Finally, to make sure there are no hard feelings, friendly, harmless Mr. Wallace comes out in coat and tie. Gives jovial greeting. Friendly reconciliation takes place. Experiment ends.) Subjects in the experiment were not asked to give the 450- volt shock more than three times. By that time, it seemed evident that they would go on indefinitely. "No one,� says Milgram, "who got within five shocks of the end ever broke off. By that point, he has resolved the conflict." Why do so many people resolve the conflict in favor of obedience? Milgram's theory assumes that people behave in two different operating modes as different as ice and water. He does not rely on Freud or sex or toilet-training hang-ups for this theory. All he says is that ordinarily we operate in a state of autonomy, which means we pretty much have and assert control over what we do. But in certain circumstances, we operate under what Milgram calls a state of agency (agent-- any one who acts for or in the place of another by authority from him; a substitute; a deputy--Webster�s Collegiate Dictionary). A state of agency, to Milgram, is nothing more than a frame of mind. "There�s nothing bad about it, there's nothing good about it," he says. "It's a natural circumstance of living with other people. I think of a state of agency as a real transformation of a person; if a person has different properties when he's in that state, just as water can turn to ice under certain conditions of temperature, a person can move to the state of mind that I call agency--the critical thing is that you see yourself as the instrument of the execution of another person's wishes. You do not see yourself as acting on your own. And there�s a real transformation, a real change of properties of the person." To achieve this change you have to be in a situation where there seems to be a ruling authority whose commands are relevant to some legitimate purpose; the authority�s power is not unlimited. But situations can be and have been structured to make people do unusual things, and not just in Milgram�s laboratory. The reason, says Milgram, is that no action, in and of itself, contains meaning. "The meaning always depends on your definition of the situation. Take an action like killing another person. It sounds bad.� "But then we say the other person was about to destroy a hundred children, and the only way to stop him was to kill him. Well, that sounds good. "Or, you take destroying your own life. It sounds very bad. Yet, in the Second World War, thousands of persons thought it was a good thing to destroy your own life. It was set in the proper context. You sipped some saki from a whistling cup, recited a few haiku. You said, 'May my death be as clean and as quick as the shattering of crystal.' And it almost seemed like a good, noble thing to do, to crash your kamikaze plane into an aircraft carrier. But the main thing was, the definition of what a kamikaze pilot was doing had been determined by the relevant authority. Now once you are in a state of agency, you allow the authority to determine, to define what the situation is. The meaning of your action is altered." So for most subjects in Milgrim�s laboratory experiments, the act of giving Mr. Wallace his painful shock was necessary, even though unpleasant, and besides they were doing it on behalf of somebody else and it was for science. There was still strain and conflict, of course. Most people resolved it by grimly sticking to their task and obeying. But some broke out. Milgram tried varying the conditions of the experiment to see what would help break people out of their state of agency. "The results, as seen and felt in the laboratory,� he has written, "are disturbing. They raise the possibility that human nature, or more specifically the kind of character produced in American democratic society, cannot be counted on to insulate its citizens from brutality and inhumane treatment at the direction of malevolent authority. A substantial proportion of people do what they are told to do, irrespective of the content of the act and without limitation of conscience, so long as they perceive that the command comes from a legitimate authority. If, in this study, an anonymous experimenter can successfully command adults to subdue a 50-year-old man and force on him painful electric shocks against his protest, one can only wonder what government, with its vastly greater authority and prestige, can command of its subjects." This is a nice statement, but it falls short of summing up the full meaning of Milgram's work. It leaves some questions still unanswered. The first question is this: Should we really be surprised and alarmed that people obey? Wouldn't it be even more alarming if they all refused to obey? Without obedience to a relevant ruling authority there could not be a civil society. An without a civil society, as Thomas Hobbes pointed out in the seventeenth century, we would live in a condition of war, "of every man against every other man," and life would be "solitary, poor, nasty, brutish and short." In the middle of one of Stanley Milgram's lectures at CUNY recently, some miniskirted undergraduates started whispering and giggling in the back of the room. He told them to cut it out. Since he was the relevant authority in that time and that place, they obeyed, and most people in the room were glad that they had obeyed. This was not, of course, a conflict situation. Nothing in the coeds� social upbringing made it a matter of conscience for them to whisper and giggle. But a case can be made that in a conflict situation it is all the more important to obey. Take the case of war, for example. Would we really want a situation in which every participant in a war, direct or indirect--from front-line soldiers to the people who sell coffee and cigarettes to employees at the Concertina barbed-wire factory in Kansas--stops and consults his conscience before each action? It is asking for an awful lot of mental strain and anguish from an awful lot of people. The value of having civil order is that one can do his duty, or whatever interests him, or whatever seems to benefit him at the moment, and leave the agonizing to others. When Francis Gary Powers was being tried by a Soviet military tribunal after his U-2 spy plane was shot down, the presiding judge asked if he had thought about the possibility that his flight might have provoked a war. Powers replied with Hobbesian clarity: "The people who sent me should think of these things. My job was to carry out orders. I do not think it was my responsibility to make such decisions." It was not his responsibility. And it is quite possible that if everyone felt responsible for each of the ultimate consequences of his own tiny contribution to complex chains of events, then society simply would not work. Milgram, fully conscious of the moral and social implications of his research, believes that people should feel responsible for their actions. If someone else had invented the experiment, and if he had been the naive subject, he feels certain that he would have been among the disobedient minority. "There is no very good solution to this,� he admits, thoughtfully. "To simply and categorically say that you won�t obey authority may resolve your personal conflict, but it creates more problems for society which may be more serious in the long run. But I have no doubt that to disobey is the proper thing to do in this [the laboratory] situation. It is the only reasonable value judgment to make." The conflict between the need to obey the relevant ruling authority and the need to follow your conscience becomes sharpest if you insist on living by an ethical system based on a rigid code--a code that seeks to answer all questions in advance of their being raised. Code ethics cannot solve the obedience problem. Stanley Milgram seems to be a situation ethicist, and situation ethics does offer a way out: When you feel conflict, you examine the situation and then make a choice among the competing evils. You may act with a presumption in favor of obedience, but reserve the possibility that you will disobey whenever obedience demands a flagrant and outrageous affront to conscience. This, by the way, is the philosophical position of many who resist the draft. In World War II, they would have fought. Vietnam is a different, an outrageously different, situation. Life can be difficult for the situation ethicist, because he does not see the world in straight lines, while the social system too often assumes such a God-given, squared-off structure. If your moral code includes an injunction against all war, you may be deferred as a conscientious objector. If you merely oppose this particular war, you may not be deferred. Stanley Milgram has his problems, too. He believes that in the laboratory situation, he would not have shocked Mr. Wallace. His professional critics reply that in his real-life situation he has done the equivalent. He has placed innocent and naive subjects under great emotional strain and pressure in selfish obedience to his quest for knowledge. When you raise this issue with Milgram, he has an answer ready. There is, he explains patiently, a critical difference between his naive subjects and the man in the electric chair. The man in the electric chair (in the mind of the naive subject) is helpless, strapped in. But the naive subject is free to go at any time. Immediately after he offers this distinction, Milgram anticipates the objection. "It�s quite true,� he says, "that this is almost a philosophic position, because we have learned that some people are psychologically incapable of disengaging themselves. But that doesn't relieve them of the moral responsibility." The parallel is exquisite. "The tension problem was unexpected," says Milgram in his defense. But he went on anyway. The naive subjects didn't expect the screaming protests from the strapped-in learner. But they went on. "I had to make a judgment," says Milgram. "I had to ask myself, was this harming the person or not? My judgment is that it was not. Even in the extreme cases, I wouldn�t say that permanent damage results." Sound familiar? "The shocks may be painful,� the experimenter kept saying, "but they're not dangerous." After the series of experiments was completed, Milgram sent a report of the results to his subjects and a questionnaire, asking whether they were glad or sorry to have been in the experiment. Eighty-three and seven-tenths percent said they were glad and only 1.3 percent were sorry; 15 percent were neither sorry nor glad. However, Milgram could not be sure at the time of the experiment that only 1.3 percent would be sorry. Kurt Vonnegut, Jr., put one paragraph in the preface to Mother Night, in 1966, which pretty much says it for the people with their fingers on the shock-generator switches, for you and me, and maybe even for Milgram. "If I'd been born in Germany,� Vonnegut said, "I suppose I would have been a Nazi, bopping Jews and gypsies and Poles around, leaving boots sticking out of snowbanks, warming myself with my sweetly virtuous insides. So it goes.� Just so. One thing happened to Milgram back in New Haven during the days of the experiment was that he kept running into people he'd watched from behind the one-way glass. It gave him a funny feeling, seeing those people going about their everyday business in New Haven and knowing what they would do to Mr. Wallace if ordered to. Now that his research results are in and you've thought about it, you can get this funny feeling too. You don't need one-way glass. A glance in your own mirror may serve just as well. So exciting! Both OP and this, worths the reading time over any goddamn manga! Thanks necrophile-chan for raising this! |
Jul 2, 2015 8:26 PM
#41
Holy necro, batman. |
More topics from this board
» If anime/manga are the only mediums you consume, how much are you missing?thewiru - 7 hours ago |
6 |
by BilboBaggins365
»»
1 minute ago |
|
» What do you think of vegans feeding vegan diets to their cats? Aren't they ironically the ones doing animal abuse?fleurbleue - Oct 7 |
24 |
by LoveYourSmile
»»
1 minute ago |
|
» What's your favorite quote?Zakatsuki_ - Sep 18 |
23 |
by XMGA030
»»
10 minutes ago |
|
» Do you actually like the food associated with your country or culture?fleurbleue - Oct 9 |
31 |
by BilboBaggins365
»»
18 minutes ago |
|
» china crossdresser are so cute omg!!!Ymir_The_Viking - 3 hours ago |
14 |
by philtecturophy
»»
18 minutes ago |